Learned Helplessness: Seligman’s Theory of Depression
The concept of learned helplessness is a cornerstone of many important theories and ideas in psychology, and it’s the basis for several foundational concepts in positive psychology.
Even outside the field of psychology, it’s pretty widely understood.
It provides an explanation for some human behaviors that might seem odd or counterproductive, and understanding learned helplessness provides pathways to removing or reducing its negative impacts.
Learned helplessness was discovered through some well-known laboratory experiments that you might have learned about in a Psychology 101 class. Those experiments were conducted using methods that likely would horrify any reasonable member of an institutional review board today.
Although it has been about 50 years since learned helplessness became a well-understood psychological theory, it still looms large in the field. If you’re interested in learning more about this important concept, you’ve come to the right place. This article will cover what learned helplessness is, what impact it can have on a person’s life, how to neutralize or reverse that impact, and how to measure one’s degree of learned helplessness.
Before you continue, we thought you might like to download our three Resilience Exercises for free. These engaging, science-based exercises will help you to effectively deal with difficult circumstances and give you the tools to improve the resilience of your clients, students, or employees.
This Article Contains:
- What Is Learned Helplessness? A Psychological Definition
- Martin Seligman’s Experiments That Led to the Theory
- Examples of Learned Helplessness in Humans
- Learned Helplessness and Depression
- It’s Most Likely to be Associated With . . .
- It’s Most Likely to Promote . . .
- Learned Helplessness in Education
- Learned Helplessness in Relationships and Domestic Violence
- Learned Helplessness: The Book
- A Possible Cure—Potential Treatments for Children and Adults
- Seligman’s Learned Optimism Model
- Relevant Tests, Scales, and Questionnaires
- Relevant YouTube Videos
- Most Interesting Research
- A Take-Home Message
- References
What Is Learned Helplessness? A Psychological Definition
Learned helplessness is a phenomenon observed in both humans and other animals when they have been conditioned to expect pain, suffering, or discomfort without a way to escape it (Cherry, 2017). Eventually, after enough conditioning, the animal will stop trying to avoid the pain at all—even if there is an opportunity to truly escape it.
When humans or other animals start to understand (or believe) that they have no control over what happens to them, they begin to think, feel, and act as if they are helpless.
This phenomenon is called learned helplessness because it is not an innate trait. No one is born believing that they have no control over what happens to them and that it is fruitless even to try gaining control. It is a learned behavior, conditioned through experiences in which the subject either truly has no control over his circumstances or simply perceives that he has no control.
Martin Seligman’s Experiments That Led to the Theory
The initial experiments that formed the basis for this theory were conducted in the late 1960s and early 1970s by psychologists Martin Seligman and Steven Maier.
These experiments will be described in detail below. Please note that some readers may find the descriptions upsetting—such experiments were more commonplace in the ’60s and ’70s, but they would likely meet lots of resistance from activists and the general public today.
Seligman and Maier were working with dogs at the time and testing their responses to electrical shocks. Some of the dogs received electrical shocks that they could not predict or control.
For this experiment, the dogs were placed in a box with two chambers divided by a low barrier. One chamber had an electrified floor and the other was not (Cherry, 2017).
When the researchers placed dogs in the box and turned on the electrified floor, they noticed a strange thing: Some dogs didn’t even attempt to jump over the low barrier to the other side. Further, the dogs who didn’t attempt to jump the barrier were generally the dogs who had previously been given shocks with no way to escape them, and the dogs who jumped the barrier tended to be those who had not received such treatment.
To further investigate this phenomenon, Seligman and Maier gathered a new batch of dogs and divided them into three groups:
- Dogs in Group One were strapped into harnesses for a period of time and were not administered any shocks;
- Dogs in Group Two were strapped into the same harnesses but were administered electrical shocks that they could avoid by pressing a panel with their noses;
- Dogs in Group Three were placed in the same harnesses and also administered electrical shocks, but were given no way to avoid them.
Once these three groups had completed this first experimental manipulation, all dogs were placed (one at a time) in the box with two chambers. Dogs from Group One and Group Two were quick to figure out that they only needed to jump over the barrier to avoid the shocks, but most of the dogs from Group Three didn’t even attempt to avoid them.
Based on their previous experience, these dogs concluded that there was nothing they could do to avoid being shocked (Seligman & Groves, 1970).
Once these results had been confirmed with dogs, Seligman and Maier conducted similar experiments on rats. Just as they did with dogs, the researchers split the rats into three groups for training: One group received escapable shocks, one received inescapable shocks, and one received no shocks at all.
The rats in the group that received escapable shocks were able to avoid shocks by pressing a lever in the box, while those in the group receiving inescapable shocks could press the lever, but would still receive shocks (Seligman & Beagley, 1975).
Later, the rats were placed in a box and received electrical shocks. A lever was present within the box that, when pressed, would allow the rats to escape the shocks.
Again, rats who were initially placed in the inescapable shock group generally did not even attempt to escape, while most of those rats in the other two groups succeeded in escaping.
The rats who did not attempt to escape were showing behavior that is classic to learned helplessness: even when presented with a potential option to avoid pain, they do not attempt to take it.
This phenomenon can also be seen in elephants. When an elephant trainer starts working with a baby elephant, he or she will use a rope to tie one of the elephant’s legs to a post. The elephant will struggle for hours, even days, trying to escape the rope, but eventually, it will quiet down and accept its range of motion (Wu, 2009).
When the elephant grows up, it will be more than strong enough to break the rope, but it won’t even try—it’s been taught that any kind of struggle is useless.
Examples of Learned Helplessness in Humans
Such extreme experiments have not been performed on humans (nor should they), the experiments that have been conducted on humans have produced similar outcomes. Although the human response to such situations may be more complex and dependent on several different factors, it still resembles the responses of dogs, rats, and other animals.
One study of learned helplessness in humans was conducted in 1974. In that study, the human participants were split into three groups: One group was subjected to a loud and unpleasant noise but was able to terminate the noise by pressing a button four times; the second group was subjected to the same noise, but the button was not functional; and the third group was subjected to no noise at all.
Later, all human participants were subjected to a loud noise and given a box with a lever which, when manipulated, would turn off the sound. Just like in the animal experiments, those who had no control over the noise in the first part of the experiment generally did not even try to turn the noise off, while the rest of the subjects generally figured out how to turn the noise off very quickly.
Seligman and colleagues proposed that subjecting participants to situations in which they have no control results in three deficits: motivational, cognitive, and emotional (Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978). The cognitive deficit refers to the subject’s idea that his circumstances are uncontrollable. The motivational deficit refers to the subject’s lack of response to potential methods of escaping a negative situation.
Finally, the emotional deficit refers to the depressed state arises when the subject is in a negative situation that he feels is not under his control.
Based on his research, Seligman found an important connection: the link between learned helplessness and depression.
Learned Helplessness and Depression
To understand the proposed connection between learned helplessness and depression, we need to understand the two types of learned helplessness, as outlined by Seligman and colleagues.
Universal helplessness is a sense of helplessness in which the subject believes nothing can be done about the situation she is in. She believes no one can alleviate the pain or discomfort.
On the other hand, personal helplessness is a much more localized sense of helplessness. The subject may believe others could find a solution or avoid the pain or discomfort, but he believes that he, personally, is incapable of finding a solution (Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978).
Both types of helplessness can lead to a state of depression, but the quality of that depression may differ. Those who feel universally helpless will tend to find external reasons for both their problems and their inability to solve them, while those who feel personally helpless will tend to find internal reasons.
In addition, those who feel personally helpless are more likely to suffer from low self-esteem since they believe others could probably solve the problems they feel incapable of solving.
Although the cognitive and motivational deficits are the same for people suffering from both personal and universal helplessness, people experiencing personal helplessness tend to have a greater and more impactful emotional deficit.
In addition to this differentiation between types of helplessness, learned helplessness can on two other factors: generality (global vs. specific) and stability (chronic vs. transient).
When a person suffers from global helplessness, they experience negative impacts in several areas of life, not just the most relevant area. They are also more likely to experience severe depression than those who experience specific helplessness.
Further, those suffering from chronic helplessness (those who have felt helpless over a long period of time) are more likely to feel the effects of depressive symptoms than those who experience transient helplessness (a short-lived and nonrecurrent sense of helplessness).
This model of learned helplessness has important implications for depression. It posits that when highly desired outcomes are believed to be improbable and/or highly aversive outcomes are believed probable, and the individual has no expectation that anything she does will change the outcome, depression results.
However, the depression will vary based on the type of helplessness. The range of depressive symptoms will depend on the generality and stability of the helplessness, and any impact on self-esteem is dependent on how the individual explains or attributes their experience (internally vs. externally).
This proposed framework identifies the cause of at least one type of depression—that which stems from helplessness—and provides the path to a cure for it. The researchers outlined four strategies for treating helplessness-related depression (Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978):
- Change the likelihood of the outcome. Alter the environment by increasing the likelihood of desired events and decreasing the likelihood of negative events;
- Reduce the desire for preferred outcomes. This can be done by either reducing the negativity of events that are outside the individual’s control or by reducing the desirability of events that are extremely unlikely to happen;
- Change the individual’s expectation from uncontrollability to controllability when the desired outcomes are attainable. In other words, help the depressed person realize when outcomes they desire are actually within their control;
- Change unrealistic explanations for failure toward those that are external (not due to some inherent flaw in the depressed person himself), transient (not chronic), and specific (due to one specific problem rather than a larger pattern of problems). Likewise, change unrealistic explanations for success to those that are internal (due to some inherent strength in the depressed person), stable (chronic), and global (due to an overall competence rather than a specific area of competence).
These strategies will be covered in more detail later.
It’s Likely to Be Associated With . . .
Learned helplessness is, unsurprisingly, associated with many negative symptoms, traits, and tendencies, including:
- Age: The older one’s age, the more likely they are to experience change or loss of roles and physical decline. Residing in an institution is also linked to learned helplessness (Foy & Mitchell, 1990);
- Stress, especially poverty-related stress (Brown, Seyler, Knorr, Garnett, & Laurenceau, 2016);
- Anxiety and worry, specifically about tests for students (Raufelder, Regner, & Wood, 2018);
- Greater negative response to anticipated pain (Strigo, Simmons, Matthews, Craig, & Paulus, 2008).
It’s Most Likely to Promote…
Not only is learned helplessness often associated with other negative conditions, but it also seems to contribute to or cause many negative outcomes, including:
- Negative health symptoms as well as negative emotions about one’s disease (Nowicka-Sauer, Hajduk, Kujawska-Danecka, Banaszkiewicz, Czuszyńska, Smoleńska, & Siebert, 2017);
- Maladaptive perfectionism (Filippello, Larcan, Sorrenti, Buzzai, Orecchio, & Costa, 2017);
- Turnover intentions (Tayfur, Karapinar, & Camgoz, 2013);
- Burnout, or emotional exhaustion and cynicism (Tayfur et al., 2013);
- Aggravated depression, anxiety, phobias, shyness, and loneliness in those already suffering (Cherry, 2017).
Learned Helplessness in Education
The topic of learned helplessness comes up quite regularly in the education field.
There’s quite a bit of interest in how early academic failure or low academic self-esteem can impact later success, and how the relationship can be influenced to enhance chances of success.
Learned helplessness in students creates a vicious cycle. Those who feel that they are unable to succeed are unlikely to put much effort into their schoolwork, which decreases their chances of success, leading to even less motivation and effort (Catapano, n.d.).
This vicious cycle may culminate in a student having virtually no motivation to learn a subject and no competence in that subject. Even worse, it could lead to a more generalized sense of helplessness in which the student has no belief in her ability and no motivation to learn any subject at school.
The reasons students give to explain their failure or success is critical in school. If a student believes he failed because the teacher hates him or he’s simply stupid, he is blaming factors that are not within his control and is likely to develop a greater sense of helplessness.
If a student believes she failed because she didn’t study hard enough, she is blaming factors that are within her control, which is much less likely to lead to an overall sense of helplessness related to school.
Luckily, there are a few strategies that can help prevent students from learning to be habitually helpless, including:
- Teachers providing praise and encouragement based on the student’s abilities (e.g., “You’re good at math” or “You have a knack for this subject, I can tell”) to help them believe they are good at these tasks or subjects;
- Teachers providing praise and encouragement based on the student’s efforts (e.g., “Your hours of hard work paid off on this test!”) to help them believe their effort will make a difference;
- Working on smart, individual goal-setting with students to help them learn that goals can be achieved and that outcomes are often within their realm of influence (Catapano, n.d.).
In addition, Edutopia’s Andrew Miller (2015) suggests a few very important strategies for teachers and parents:
- Curate and create learning resources (which include people, books, websites, and community organizations, among other resources) to help students become comfortable with not knowing the answer and with looking for the answer in the right places;
- Use questions for learning rather than about learning (e.g., use questions that encourage the student to think about his own learning and thought patterns instead of just thinking about what he knows);
- Stop giving students the answers. Instead, help them learn it at their own pace and through their own methods—they’ll be more likely to remember it this way!
- Allow them to fail. Failing and trying again is vital for children—as long as you are there to support them when they fail.
In addition to these strategies, later on in this piece we’ll discuss some insights into treating or “curing” learned helplessness that can be applied to students.
Learned Helplessness in Relationships and Domestic Violence
In addition to education, learned helplessness comes up often for people focused on domestic violence. It’s often observed in relationships and in victims of domestic violence.
In fact, this phenomenon has helped us find an answer to some of the questions people have for victims who stay with their abusers, such as:
- Why didn’t they tell someone?
- Why didn’t they try to get help?
- Why didn’t they just leave?
It’s hard to explain the impact of abuse on the victim’s behavior. After all, observers might think it makes no sense that victims choose to stay with someone who is hurting them.
However, in cases of domestic violence and abuse, abusers often administer a series of “electrical shocks” (i.e., the form of abuse they subject their victims to) to acclimatize the victims to the abuse and teach them that they do not have control over the situation. The abusers maintain complete control, and the victims learn that they are helpless about their circumstances.
In such cases, it is easy to see how abuse can lead to learned helplessness, which can subsequently lead to a lack of motivation or effort to escape on the victim’s part. Just as the dogs in Seligman and Maier’s experiments learned that no matter what they did, they would be shocked, the victims of domestic violence and abuse learn that no matter what they do, they will always remain powerless and under the abusers’ control.
These perceptions are incredibly hard to shake, often requiring intensive therapy and support in order to shake them.
Based on learned helplessness, a specific theory was developed for victims of domestic violence called the theory of cyclic abuse, a cycle that is sometimes known as battered women syndrome. In this theory, a relationship in which domestic violence has occurred is likely to continually involve violence that’s doled out in a predictable and repetitious pattern.
This pattern generally follows this structure:
- Stage One: a period of tension-building in which the abuser starts to get angry, communication breaks down, and the victim feels the need to concede and submit to the abuser;
- Stage Two: the acting-out period, in which the abuse occurs;
- Stage Three: the honeymoon period, in which the abuser may apologize, show remorse, and/or try to make up for the abuse. The abuser might also promise never to abuse the victim again or, alternatively, blame the victim for provoking the abuse;
- Stage Four: the calm period, in which the abuse stops, the abuser acts like it never happened, and the victim may start to believe the abuse has ended and the abuser will change (Rakovec-Felser, 2014).
Viewed from this perspective, it’s not surprising that many victims of domestic violence develop learned helplessness. When the abuse is inflicted upon them in a continuing cycle, no matter what they do, they are likely to feel completely helpless and unable to avoid the abuse.
The theory of cyclic abuse posits that not only will abuse victims feel helpless, they will also:
- Re-experience the battering as if it were recurring even when it is not;
- Attempt to avoid the psychological impact of battering by avoiding activities, people, and emotions;
- Experience hyperarousal or hypervigilance;
- Have disrupted interpersonal relationships;
- Experience body image distortion or other somatic concerns;
- Develop sexuality and intimacy issues (Rakovec-Felser, 2014).
Clearly, learned helplessness is a serious and urgent concern for victims of domestic violence and other abuse. Luckily, there are some ways to treat learned helplessness (see the section on treatments).
Learned Helplessness: The Book
The book Learned Helplessness: A Theory for the Age of Personal Control was written by psychologist Christopher Peterson along with the first researchers to study learned helplessness, Maier and Seligman.
It chronicles the studies that prompted the theory of learned helplessness and provides a cogent and comprehensive summary of the research up to the book’s publication (in 1995) on the phenomenon. It outlines the connection between learned helplessness and depression as well as investigating other facets, like the cognitive and biological aspects.
If you are looking for a deeper dive into this topic, this book will provide you with an informative overview of learned helplessness. You can find it for purchase here.
A Possible Cure—Potential Treatments for Children and Adults
While learned helplessness can be hard to overcome, there exist promising treatments to address it in humans (and in other animals, for that matter).
One potential treatment based on neuroscience research is the relationship between the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (a part of the brain that plays a role in the inhibition of emotional responses) and the dorsal raphe nucleus (a part of the brainstem associated with serotonin and depression) and learned helplessness (Maier & Seligman, 2016).
This potential treatment may focus on stimulating the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and inhibiting the dorsal raphe nucleus through medication, electrical stimulation, or trans-magnetic stimulation, or psychologically through therapy.
Trans-magnetic stimulation (TMS) in particular has been shown in recent studies to be quite effective in the treatment of depression (Mayo Clinic, 2017). Given the link between learned helplessness and depression, it makes sense to think that a treatment for one may be an effective treatment for the other.
Speaking of effective treatments for depression, therapy is also a good choice for people struggling with learned helplessness. Those who feel helpless can benefit from working with a licensed mental health professional to explore the origins of their helplessness, replace old and harmful beliefs with newer and healthier beliefs, and develop a healing sense of compassion for themselves (Thompson, 2010).
Insightful research from psychologist Carol Dweck (the researcher who went on to propose the theory of growth vs. fixed mindset) showed that there is another extremely effective way to alleviate learned helplessness: through failure.
In Dweck’s 1975 study on the subject, participants (who all experienced extreme reactions to failure) were split into two groups: one received intensive training in which they failed tasks and were instructed to take responsibility for their failure and attribute it to a lack of effort, while the other group received intensive training in which they only experienced success.
The results showed that those in the success-only treatment group showed no improvement in their extreme reactions to failure, while the group that failed showed a marked improvement.
This experiment was one of several studies throughout the 1970s, 1980s, and 1990s that laid the foundation for a new theory of human behavior related to failure, learned helplessness, and resilience.
Seligman’s Learned Optimism Model
Seligman—one of the researchers who helped discover the learned helplessness phenomenon—later found his attention drawn to what is perhaps the complete opposite of learned helplessness: optimism.
Although Seligman’s name was synonymous with learned helplessness for many years, he knew he had a lot more to offer the world. His work on the subject led him to wonder what other mindsets and perspectives can be learned and whether people could develop positive traits instead of developing feelings of helplessness.
Seligman’s research led him to create the model of learned optimism. He found that, through resilience training, people can learn to develop a more optimistic perspective. This ability has been observed in children, teachers, members of the military, and more (Seligman, 2011).
It might not be as easy to learn optimism as it is to learn helplessness, but it can be done. If you’re interested in learning more about optimism and how it can be learned, check out Seligman’s book Learned Optimism: How to Change Your Mind and Your Life at this link. In addition to getting a brief overview of the research on this subject, you will also read about several simple techniques you can apply to develop a more positive and self-compassionate explanatory style.
Relevant Tests, Scales, and Questionnaires
Although many people have included measures of learned helplessness in their studies, they are often informal measures. However, there are two measures that have been used fairly often and/or recently.
The Learned Helplessness Scale (LHS) was developed by Quinless and Nelson (1988) to capture and calculate a score for learned helplessness. The scale is composed of 20 items rated on a scale from 1 (strongly agree) to 4 (strongly disagree). The minimum score on this measure is 20 and the maximum score is 80, with higher scores indicating a greater degree of learned helplessness.
The Learned Helplessness Questionnaire (LHQ) was created in Sorrenti and colleagues’ 2014 study on learned helplessness and mastery orientation. The LHQ consists of 13 items rated on a scale from 1 (not true) to 5 (absolutely true), for a total possible score between 13 and 65. An example item from this scale is the statement, “When you encounter an obstacle in schoolwork you get discouraged and stop trying. You are easily frustrated.”
If you’d like to use any of these scales for research purposes, please refer to the original scale development article for more information.
Relevant YouTube Videos
There are a number of great talks on learned helplessness and/or learned optimism for you to peruse.
For example, Martin Seligman’s TED Talk titled The New Era of Positive Psychology has become a classic, and for good reason. You can watch it here:
There’s also a great video on YouTube from psychologist Lance Luria on the differences between learned helplessness and learned optimism. You’ll learn about the amazing ability of the human brain to train itself, as well as the benefits of meditation, mindfulness, and other ways to link the health of the mind and body.
For an engaging and entertaining look at Seligman’s book Learned Optimism, check out the video below. It’s an animated review of the book that hits all the salient points in under five minutes.
Most Interesting Research
It’s been about five decades since the very first studies on learned helplessness, but there is still interesting new research coming out on the subject.
For instance, in 2017 researchers discovered that, although learned helplessness has been observed in honey bees, they don’t display the “freezing” behavior that other species do (Dinges, Varnon, Cota, Slykerman, & Abramson).
In 2016, researchers in Brazil found some evidence that even zebrafish experience learned helplessness (do Nascimento, Walsh-Monteiro, & Gouveia).
Not even the simple tree shrew is safe from the effects of learned helplessness—research from 2016 confirmed the presence of such behavior in tree shrews who received uncontrollable shocks to the foot (Meng, Shen, Li, Li, & Wang, 2016).
In terms of more broadly applicable research on learned helplessness, many recent experiments are probing the link between learned helplessness and the brain.
An oft-cited study from researchers Kim and colleagues (2016) showed that brain activity in mice displaying non-helpless behavior was generally much higher than that of the helpless mice. However, this pattern was reversed in the part of the brain known as the locus coeruleus, which is involved in physiological responses to stress and panic.
This finding is interesting, as it suggests that individuals experiencing learned helplessness are directing their energy toward responding to their own distress, while more resilient individuals keep their energy more normally distributed.
Research on the cellular basis of learned helplessness-related depression has shown that increased activity of the lateral habenula neurons (an area of the brain involved in communications between the forebrain and midbrain structures) in rats is associated with increased learned helplessness behavior (Li, Piriz, Mirrione, Chung, Proulx, Schulz, Henn, & Malinow, 2011).
The implications of connecting learned helplessness to activity in specific parts of the brain are potentially huge; these findings could contribute to new and more effective methods of treating and preventing depression.
This is the kind of exciting research that is happening right now—research that could have huge impacts on treating disorders and healing those who have suffered. Keep an eye out for the fascinating findings that continue to result from this line of research.
A Take-Home Message
In this piece, we defined learned helplessness, went over the experiments that laid the foundation for the theory, discussed the known associations and outcomes of learned helplessness, and dove into potential treatments for this harmful condition, including strategies to build learned optimism instead of helplessness.
If this piece sparked your curiosity about the subject that goes beyond this piece, we encourage you to check out the sources referenced here in greater detail.
What are your thoughts on learned helplessness? Do you recognize some symptoms in yourself or in your clients? How do you usually address it? Let us know your thoughts in the comments.
We hope you enjoyed reading this article. Don’t forget to download our three Resilience Exercises for free.
- Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49-74.
- Brown, E. D., Seyler, M. D., Knorr, A. M., Garnett, M. L., & Laurenceau, J. (2016). Daily poverty-related stress and coping: Associations with child learned helplessness. Family Relations, 65, 591-602.
- Catapano, J. (n.d.). Learned helplessness, and how we can overcome it. TeachHUB Hot Tips & Topics. Retrieved from http://www.teachhub.com/learned-helplessness-and-how-we-can-overcome-it
- Cherry, K. (2014). What is learned helplessness and why does it happen? VeryWell Mind. Retrieved from https://www.verywellmind.com/what-is-learned-helplessness-2795326
- Dinges, C. W., Varnon, C. A., Cota, L. D., Slykerman, S., & Abramson, C. I. (2017). Studies of learned helplessness in honey bees (Apis mellifera ligustica). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition, 43, 147-158.
- do Nascimento, G. S., Walsh-Monteiro, A., & Gouveia, A. J. (2016). A reliable depression-like model in zebrafish (Danio rerio): Learned helplessness. Psychology & Neuroscience, 9, 390-397.
- Dweck, C. (1975). The role of expectations and attributions in the alleviation of learned helplessness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 674-685.
- Filippello, P., Larcan, R., Sorrenti, L., Buzzai, C., Orecchio, S., & Costa, S. (2017). The Mediating Role of Maladaptive Perfectionism in the Association between Psychological Control and Learned Helplessness. Improving Schools, 20, 113-126.
- Foy, S. S., & Mitchell, M. M. (1990). Factors contributing to learned helplessness in the institutionalized aged: A literature review. Physical & Occupational Therapy in Geriatrics, 9, 1-23.
- Kim, Y., Perova, Z., Mirrione, M. M., Pradhan, K., Henn, F. A., Shea, S., Osten, P., & Li, B. (2016). Whole-brain mapping of neuronal activity in the learned helplessness model of depression. Neural Circuits, 10.
- Li, B., Piriz, J., Mirrione, M., Chung, C., Proulx, C. D., Schulz, D., Henn, F., & Manilow, R. (2011). Synaptic potentiation onto habenula neurons in the learned helplessness model of depression. Nature, 470, 535-539.
- Maier, S. F., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2016). Learned helplessness at fifty: Insights from neuroscience. Psychological Review, 123, 349-367.
- Mayo Clinic. (2017). Transcranial magnetic stimulation. Mayo Clinic – Tests & Procedures. Retrieved from https://www.mayoclinic.org/tests-procedures/transcranial-magnetic-stimulation/about/pac-20384625
- Meng, X., Shen, F., Li, C., Li, Y., & Wang, X. (2016). Depression-like behaviors in tree shrews and comparison of the effects of treatment with fluoxetine and carbetocin. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 145, 1-8.
- Miller, A. (2015). Avoiding “learned helplessness”. Edutopia – Teaching Strategies. Retrieved from https://www.edutopia.org/blog/avoiding-learned-helplessness-andrew-miller
- Nowicka-Sauer, K., Hajduk, A., Kujawska-Danecka, H., Banaszkiewicz, D., Czuszyńska, Z., Smoleńska, Z., & Siebert, J. (2017). Learned helplessness and its associations with illness perception, depression and anxiety among patients with systemic lupus erythematosus. Family Medicine & Primary Care Review, 19, 243-246.
- Quinless, F. W., & Nelson, M. M. (1988). Development of a measure of learned helplessness. Nursing Research, 37, 11-15.
- Rakovec-Felser, Z. (2014). Domestic violence and abuse in intimate relationships from public health perspective. Health Psychology Research, 6, 1821-1826.
- Raufelder, D., Regner, N., & Wood, M. A. (2018). Test anxiety and learned helplessness is moderated by student perceptions of teacher motivational support. Educational Psychology, 38, 54-74.
- Seligman, M. E. P. (2011). Building resilience. Harvard Business Review. Retrieved from https://hbr.org/2011/04/building-resilience
- Seligman, M. E. P., & Beagley, G. (1975). Learned helplessness in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 88, 534-541.
- Seligman, M. E. P., & Groves, D. P. (1970). Nontransient learned helplessness. Psychonomic Science, 19, 191-192.
- Sorrenti, L., Filippello, P., Costa, S., & Buzzai, C. (2014). Preliminary evaluation of a self-report tool for learned helplessness and mastery orientation in Italian students. Mediterranean Journal of Clinical Psychology, 2.
- Strigo, I. A., Simmons, A. N., Matthews, S. C., Craig, A. D., & Paulus, M. P. (2008). Association of major depressive disorder with altered functional brain response during anticipation and processing of heat pain. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65, 1275-1284.
- Tayfur, O., Karapinar, P. B., & Camgoz, S. M. (2013). The mediating effects of emotional exhaustion cynicism and learned helplessness on organizational justice-turnover intentions linkage. International Journal of Stress Management, 20, 193-221.
- Thompson, J. (2010). Learned helplessness: You’re not trapped. GoodTherapy. Retrieved from https://www.goodtherapy.org/blog/therapy-learned-helplessness/
- Wu, W. (2009, February 8). Learned helplessness: How to tame a baby elephant. [Personal Blog]. Retrieved from https://waynewu.wordpress.com/2009/02/08/learned-helplessness/
Let us know your thoughts
Read other articles by their category
- Body & Brain (41)
- Coaching & Application (49)
- Compassion (27)
- Counseling (46)
- Emotional Intelligence (23)
- Gratitude (16)
- Grief & Bereavement (19)
- Happiness & SWB (35)
- Meaning & Values (26)
- Meditation (21)
- Mindfulness (42)
- Motivation & Goals (42)
- Optimism & Mindset (33)
- Positive CBT (24)
- Positive Communication (21)
- Positive Education (41)
- Positive Emotions (28)
- Positive Psychology (33)
- Positive Workplace (38)
- Relationships (31)
- Resilience & Coping (33)
- Self Awareness (21)
- Self Esteem (38)
- Software & Apps (23)
- Strengths & Virtues (28)
- Stress & Burnout Prevention (27)
- Theory & Books (42)
- Therapy Exercises (30)
- Types of Therapy (53)
What our readers think
Could someone please direct me to the Learned Helplessness Scale (LSH) developed by Quinless and Nelson (1988). I’ve gone through their article “Development of a Measure of Learned Helplessness” but did not find the scale items mentioned in it (atleast not in the version I read). I am planning on researching on learned helplessness and need access to the scale. If anyone can link me to it, I would highly appreciate it. Thanks.
Hi Aymen,
Thanks for your question. The scale is definitely a bit tricky to find. After some digging, I found the scale in the Appendix of this paper.
I hope this helps.
Kind regards,
-Caroline | Community Manager
you state that experiments, like the dog, rat and elephant training haven’t and never should be carried out on humans yet I immediately thought of slavery especially the Black American experience. Millions of people were trained or broken and there’s a possibility that learned helplessness is still carried out in our present society.
Just a thought.
While reading through this article, I had the exact same question
Hi MC and Jed,
I think you’re right. Learned helplessness as a psychological phenomenon was only conceptualised by Seligman in the late ’60s to early ’70s, long after slavery (at least in the United States) was obviously abolished. While there’s a lot of work looking at the horrific toll of unethical medical (physical) experiments that were conducted on slaves, there appears to be much less published about psychological experiments and their consequences. There appears to be a dearth of research on the psychological experience of life as a slave in general (see Stephens, 1999).
Nonetheless, I’m sure the experience of learned helplessness was pervasive, and likely persists among populations who continue to experience oppression today. But the psychological process of learned helplessness that was occurring was not well understood until relatively recently, so it is not always referred to as such.
– Nicole | Community Manager
I thought of seat belts and the fact that even strollers have shoulder belts nowadays.
(At least a slave has the hope of freedom if he can run far enough. Where do you go if cops the world over are waiting to pull you over and harass you for being able to jump to the other side of the car?)
I am a Native American, and this is true for generations of us.
Great ! Just great great stuff. If I’ve a complaint – and I could be wrong – I think I noticed a grammar error or two in the body of the article. Still, I’m now working on the links & questionnaires..
Jef (K)irby, 12 step guy
Hi Jef,
So glad you found the article useful! Could you please let us know where abouts you spotted the grammatical errors? We’d love to fix these up.
Thanks!
– Nicole | Community Manager
Brilliant article. Living in a “custodial type care institution” will result in learned helplessness in mental hospitals it is as if by design they want you to “break” and aren’t happy until they see you docile and complies the. I learned though they do not have your best interests at heart. We are subjected to trauma there inside and gaslighting outside. Helplessness translates to hopelessness.
Learning of helplessness are important and necessary for me to help my family’s.
thanks so much
I want to thank you for this well-written introducing article. I made some important study-related and personal insights, and the theory itself fits perfectly into my student research. Thank you!
I wrote about this as a psychology undergraduate, citing Seligman’s work and how the concept can be applied to entire countries as a method of control and to alter the collective consciousness of, as we see in the latest bout of abuse with the covid fiasco, the entire world. I am in absolutely no doubt whatsoever about this.
I also suggested that the sense of psychological and physical unpleasantness produced during the process of learning that one is helpless, is the result cognitive dissonance as our sense of self and our core values are altered. This results in decline of health (I referenced Finnish research in to declining levels of health in humans and higher primates under chronic stress and a pessimistic perception of future events and outcomes). In relation to this, I proposed a study for my final year project about how learned helplessness and resultant cognitive dissonance and its secondary effects were at the core of Scotland’s health problems as well as its continued submission to English rule.
My then academic supervisor, Dr. C.W, rejected my proposal for the study I wanted to undertake related to this topic saying that it was a PhD level study and that I would fail my degree if I tried to take it on. I agreed (probably correctly) and did paper on some reasoning stuff that I didn’t care too much about. I got my degree. Didn’t solve Scotland’s health issue though.
I feel like my spouse does this to me and I am in this state. I don’t know what to do, but nothing seems reasonable
It’s affecting everything in life
observe your spouse’s behaviour over time – maybe make notes to summarise and reflect
if they are socio/psychopathic/machiavellian manipulator/predatory they may prefer to gaslight you so you end up thinking you are the crazy one, and it’s all your fault.
talk to detached/unrelated observers about your spouse’s behaviour – do they look wonderful in public, and treat you bad in private ? How do they treat waiters – that tends to be a real clue about respect or disdain for others.
if you think they won’t change, make plans to leave.
if you think they can and will be willing to change, choose a time when they are relaxed and in a good mood (not hangry, not stressed, maybe after an evening meal when you’re both sitting quietly and comfortably, not about to do anything else, NOT drunk) then state simply and quietly that you’re not happy about some aspects of your relationship and you’d like to discuss it with them. Keep it flat mild voice – not emotional. And wait for their response.
If they react by arcing up and getting angry – there’s a clue – they’re bad for you.
If they say ‘sure – what would you like to talk about’ – there’s an opportunity.
So close to what I’m in. He treats everyone, but me, with friendliness etc, even waiters. I’m also in chronic pain and disabled and he had to change his life doe me to,get the health care I need. I get no hopeful messages when I’m down, Just silence. No touching, no a’ hang in there”, no nothing. He feels, I think, if he feeds me and has a roof over my head and pay for my health care, he’s done his part. I feel blame every day.
I have been interested in this concept for many years. I am not an academic nor a psychologist however I am an observer of human behaviour and someone who grew up in the social services system. A number of years ago someone conducted a longitudinal study that I believe was focused on youth in care and/or had committed illegal acts. An over simplification however the conclusion was that some people seem to be born with resilience while others are not. The author did not say resilience could not be learned or enhanced but it seemed to explain why among children born into similar circumstances some thrived while others did not. Perhaps that belief has been refuted although I cannot see that is the case here. One of my closely held beliefs is best explained by Mark Twain who said There are lies, damn lies and then there are statistics.
What is the citation for Cherry 2017 please? If it is the VeryWellMind blog, it seems to have been updated – https://www.verywellmind.com/what-is-learned-helplessness-2795326 (Please delete the other comment with my last name thanks).